
(Photo: Jirka Matousek © CC BY 2.0. Edited.)
This is a translation from Danish of the May 2023 update of a chapter in my series, “The Quest for the Greek Language”, about learning Greek from a Danish perspective. A few changes and additions were made to accommodate English grammar, notably in the section listing various syntagmata.
Introduction
People wanting to learn a new language might easily be overwhelmed by having to consider the language’s many details and then figure out how to learn everything.
Language is many things. That’s why linguistics describes languages through different perspectives, each having its own name.
This dictionary provides you with an overview of basic concepts within central linguistic disciplines, to help you manage language learning. It also introduces you to sentence analysis.
Knowing this vocabulary makes it much easier to find and use facts about specific languages. You’ll learn to understand and make use of the descriptions found in grammar books.
Use this text as a reference while learning a foreign language. You can also read the whole thing to get a full overview of topics, or just choose the parts you’re interested in.
Table of Contents
- Introduction
- How a language is pronounced: Phonology
- How words are built: Morphology
- How sentences are built: Syntax
- Language in context: Pragmatics
- Linguistic variety
- Bibliography
How a language is pronounced: Phonology
Phonology is knowledge about how speech sounds are used in a specific language. It relies on concepts from phonetics, which is knowledge about how humans produce speech sounds in general.
The concepts in this section mainly come from phonetics, but I’ve written “phonology” in the title because you’re supposed to use them to learn the phonology of the language you’re interested in.
Phonemes
The primary speech sounds are called phonemes. These are the smallest elements of language used for distinguishing between meanings.
One example could be the difference betweeen the words “red” and “rad”. We know they have different meanings because they sound different.
Phonemes don’t have their own, built⁠-⁠in meaning. They’re not “carriers of meaning”.
Phonemes are sometimes pronounced differently in specific contexts. These different varieties are called allophones. These stand in place of phonemes and, as such, are incapable of distinguishing between meanings in their own right. They just distinguish the way the original phoneme did.
Speaking is the act of using the speech organs in your mouth and throat (such as the tongue, palate, and pharynx) to produce a flow of speech enabled by the air you exhale.
A phonological or phonetic analysis consists of listening to segments of spoken language, transcribing what you hear into the phonetic alphabet, and identifying what is going on in that pronunciation.
Speech consists of sequences of grouped phonemes known as syllables.
Phonemes are separated into 3 different categories: vowels, glides, and consonants. These play different roles within syllables:
-
Vowels (such as [a], [e], [i], [o]) sound like pure tones. These make up the core element in syllables. They can stand alone, and it’s possible to have a syllable that only consists of a single vowel.
Some syllables feature combinations of vowels. The vowels always stand right next to each other and are pronounced in transition into one another. (But they are not what is known as a “glide”, see below.)
When 2 vowels are combined, it’s called a diphthong. When 3 are combined, it’s a triphthong. -
Glides (such as [j] and [w] in the words “yes” and “west”) are also known as semi-consonants or semi-vowels. They sort of sound like vowel combinations, but are a single phoneme that is pronounced more rapidly.
This makes them incapable of creating syllables on their own. They’re added on either side of the vowel (or combination of vowels). -
Consonants (such as [b], [g], [p], [k], [t]) can sound very different. There are many types, but they’re all produced by using your speech organs to create obstacles that prevent the air you exhale from flowing freely. (When the air flows freely, it produces a vowel.)
Consonants are also called “co⁠-⁠sounds” because they can’t create syllables on their own. They’re added on either side of the vowel (or combination of vowels).
There can be multiple consonants and glides on either side of the vowel (or combination of vowels). To summarize, within syllables, consonants and glides typically play second fiddle.
(Syllable-forming consonants do exist, but they’re an exception only found in some languages.)
Utterances
A sequence of speech is called an utterance no matter what it represents –⁠ a word, a sentence, grammatically incorrect language, etc.
Utterances are the sum of phonemes in a sequence of speech.
Prosody
Some effects in pronunciation go beyond syllables. They can occur between syllables or even groups of syllables. These phenomena are referred to as prosody.
As a field of inquiry, prosody focuses on things like how syllables are combined into rhythm groups, or when and how stress is used. These effects vary between languages.
(Grammar books will typically describe the phonemes and allophones used in a language, but not the prosody.)
Accent
An accent is a way of pronouncing a language using phonemes and prosody that don’t belong to that language.
As such, accents stand in contrast to how the ‘norm’ is idealized. Don’t worry about having an accent while learning to speak a new language. You can always change and develop it. The most important thing is that native speakers can understand you.
Why learn phonology? Pronunciation versus the alphabet
About 3,000 years ago, people in Ancient Greece adopted and modified the Phoenician abjad, which is a writing system where each character represents a consonant (Bakker 2019). It had no characters for vowels.
The characters for those Phoenician consonants that didn’t have equivalents in the Ancient Greek language were changed into representing vowels (Arndt 2009, 52⁠–⁠53). This resulted in an alphabet, which is a writing system with individual characters for both vowels and consonants.
“As far as we know, the Greek alphabet was the first to make the step towards independently indicating vowels.” (Arndt 2009, 53)
Alphabets typically lack characters for diphthongs, triphthongs, and glides. They also tend to exclusively feature phonemes, but not allophones. Moreover, the way a language is pronounced is subject to linguistic variety.
For example, Greek orthography remains “largely unchanged since antiquity, whereas the speech system has obviously changed” (Engberg 1995, 14).
The current Greek alphabet has several letters for the same vowel, [i]: η, ι, υ. These used to be three different phonemes.
The allophones [b], [d], [g] don’t have letters. These are written by combining two letters: μπ, ντ, γκ. Such combinations are called digraphs.
The discrepancies between alphabets and how languages are actually pronounced makes it important to understand the relationship between the two in the language you’re trying to learn. You can’t do this without phonology.
The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA)
A special writing system has been designed to make it easier to precisely describe speech in writing. This system is called the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA).
A phonetic transcription is when you write down the precise pronunciation heard in a segment of speech, in IPA.
The complexity of IPA is both an advantage and a disadantage for language learners. Let me explain using an example from my time at the university:
I studied French and German as applied languages. Whenever I met linguistics majors who had chosen to minor in a specific language, they were clearly better at transcribing precisely and in great detail. I was also taught how to transcribe correctly, but with a lower degree of detail than them.
It's an advantage for linguistics majors to know phonetic script at a high level. That enables them to study pronunciation in many languages with a lot of detail.
But it would have been a disadvantage for us students of specific languages to spend much time learning all of the IPA, which can hypothetically accommodate any language in the world. Our courses only focused on the parts found in the languages we were studying.
It’s difficult to learn the rules for how a language is pronounced if you can’t understand written descriptions of the pronunciation. IPA is used everywhere in non⁠-⁠fiction about languages.
That’s why I recommend learning the basic principles of IPA, focusing on the phonemes and allophones used in the language you’re interested in. You can find these in a grammar book.
But don’t focus exclusively on IPA while learning. Imagine being asked to pronounce the “unvoiced, dental fricative consonant” /⁠θ⁠/ without having a person to help you get started.
You should also spend time on listening, practicing, learning from audiovisual content, and perhaps taking in⁠-⁠person lessons.
How words are built: Morphology
Morphology is about words and their forms, including how new words are created and how they’re inflected to convey grammatical information.
Morphemes
The basic building blocks in words are called morphemes. These are the smallest units in language that both are “carriers of meaning” and have their own, independent form.
A morphological analysis focuses on identifying and classifying the different morphemes found within a given word.
Here are the three most important types of morphemes according to Nølke (2014, 5):
Root morphemes
Roots express the word’s basic meaning. There are two types:
- Semantic roots (or content words) refer to things “out in the real world”. Examples include nouns and verbs.
- Function words express things that are internal to the language system. Examples include pronouns and prepositions.
Word classes (parts of speech)
Root morphemes can always be classified as one of the parts of speech, which is a category of words sharing the same basic features. These categories are also called word classes, which will be the term used throughout this dictionary.
Nølke (2014, 6) has provided “a practical classification”, reproduced here with a few additions:
- Nouns, including common nouns and proper nouns
- Pronouns –⁠ there are a lot of subcategories
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Prepositions
- Conjunctions, including coordinating and subordinating ones. (Some people call the former “conjunctions” and the latter “subjunctions”.)
- True adverbs (meaning those that aren’t the result of derivation)
- Numerals, including cardinal and ordinal numbers
- Interjections
- Onomatopoeia
The items on this list aren’t defined here, but you can find definitions in online dictionaries such as The Free Dictionary. Moreover, the list above isn’t exhaustive.
Nominal units are an umbrella term for nouns, pronouns, adjectives, and numerals.
Open and closed word classes
- Open word classes are those that often have new words added to them. This is particularly true of nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs. (These are all “semantic roots”, or content words.)
- Closed word classes are those that (largely) never have words added to them. Their lexicons consist of short lists of specific words –⁠ and no more. This is particularly true of pronouns, conjunctions, and prepositions. (These are all function words.)
Derivational and inflectional morphemes
There exist two central types of morpheme that can be ‘attached’ to root morphemes. These have specific purposes.
- Derivational morphemes either change the word’s final meaning or convert it from one word class to another (which also causes a slight change in meaning).
-
Inflectional morphemes express the categories of information that determine the word’s grammatical form in any given situation. The specific categories vary by word class. There are two additional subtypes of inflection (but they all belong to the same category of morpheme):
- Conjugation refers to the inflection of verbs.
- Declination refers to the inflection of nominal units (nouns, pronouns, adjectives, and numerals).
Affixes: Prefixes and suffixes
There are different terms for indicating the relative position of derivational and inflectional morphemes within a word. In English, these positions are generally known as affixes.
The two most common ones are prefixes (positioned before the root) and suffixes (positioned after it), but there are others.
Since it’s typically impossible to move the same morpheme between different positions, it’s normal to say that affixes “are” their position. You would probably say “That’s a suffix” rather than “It’s in the suffix position”.
For example, consider the words “swiftness”, “eagerness”, and “silliness”. Each root morpheme (“swift”, “eager”, “silly”) is an adjective. In English, the suffix -⁠ness is a derivational morpheme that you add to an adjective to convert the final word into a noun.
The morpheme -⁠ness cannot appear as a prefix. It would make no sense to try saying “nessswift”, “nesseager”, or “nesssilli”.
In dictionaries, suffixes are written with a hyphen in front of them to indicate that something must stand before them. Prefixes are written with a hyphen after them.
The derivational morphemes that only change the word’s meaning, but not its class, are often prefixes, such as un⁠- in the word “untamable”.
Finite and non⁠-⁠finite verbs
Within conjugation (the inflection of verbs), the terms “finite” and “non⁠-⁠finite” refer to different subsections of the many different grammatical forms a verb can take depending on the situation.
In other words, a verb such as “(to) read” comes in both finite and non⁠-⁠finite forms.
-
Finite verbs realize the sentence’s main verbal function and enter into a nexus relation with the subject. Finite verbs are what establishes a sentence, or main clause. They typically have tense and grammatical mood as categories in their conjugation.
Example: “I understand it.” (A finite verb in the present tense realizing the verbal function in a main clause.) -
Non⁠-⁠finite verbs don’t enter into nexus relations. They always play a subordinate role, typically (but not exclusively) as the main verb in a subordinate clause. They typically don’t have tense or grammatical mood as categories in their conjungation.
Examples:- “Mary likes to read.” (The infinitive, used here for realizing a direct object.)
- “Roleplaying is fun." (The gerund, used here for realizing a subject.)
Some non⁠-⁠finite forms are also used for creating “complex” finite forms. Consider the sentence, “The lawyers have studied the Constitution.” The two words make up a finite tense: the present perfect. But the second word is the past participle of the verb “study”. Participles are non⁠-⁠finite forms.
Much more could be said about this topic, but my purpose here is simply to inform about the difference between finite and non⁠-⁠finite forms.
Analytic and synthetic languages
This section’s topic is one of the main typological differences between languages at the morphological level.
- Analytic languages completely or mostly express grammatical information by combining independent words. This requires having many function words.
-
Synthetic languages completely or mostly express grammatical information through derivation and inflection. This requires a larger number of morphemes to modify the way words are formed.
There are two subcategories of synthetic languages:- Fusional languages (or inflecting languages) express grammatical categories in a single, complex morpheme.
- Agglutinating languages express grammatical categories spread across multiple morphemes within the same word.
Most languages in Europe have both analytic and synthetic (fusional/inflecting) features, but they will lean more towards one or the other.
English, French, and Danish are comparatively analytic languages. German and Greek are more synthetic than those. Getting used to these differences can be a challenge during the learning process.
How sentences are built: Syntax
Syntax is about a language’s rules for creating sentences. The way sentences are systematized determines the words and forms you can use in a given situation.
When is it a word?
Words are “carriers of meaning” and have their own form. Here, I use the word “meaning” to refer to dictionary definitions, not meaning in a broader, context⁠-⁠rooted, communicative sense.
Even so, Herslund (2015) argues that it’s difficult to find the precise boundary for what words are and what they aren’t.
Root morphemes can stand on their own within sentences. Those always belong to a word class (part of speech), so they must surely be words.
On the other hand, derivational and inflectional morphemes depend on roots and don’t belong to a word class. Those aren’t words.
And yet the product of combining different types of morphemes into a single unit is typically referred to as a word because it can stand on its own in sentences. This is made possible by its root morpheme, which is the founding unit of the word.
The idea that words can always stand on their own is challenged by the phenomenon of enclisis. This happens when a function word “melts together” with another word within the same phrase.
Clitics are units that behave like independent words at the sentence level but are actually bound to another word at other levels of language –⁠ especially the phonological level, where the clitic word usually has a very brief pronunciation.
Examples in English include the constructs “it’s” and “you’re”. Each of these represents two independent words that have become bound to one another and have merged at the phonological level. “It” and “you” are both function words (personal pronouns), and here they have merged with a finite verb, “is” and “are”, respectively.
In these examples, the enclisis is highlighted by the use of apostrophes between the two words. This is not always the case in every language or situation.
Here’s an example from Modern Greek:
Greek has no dative case. When you want to use an indirect object with a definite article,there’s enclisis between the preposition σε and the definite article in the accusative case.
These are written without highlighting enclisis whatsoever –⁠ στο(ν), στη(ν), or στο –⁠ easily giving the (incorrect) impression that these are single words. They represent σε plus either το(ν), τη(ν), or το.
Sentence analysis
Sentences are created by combining words in specific, rule⁠-⁠bound ways.
A sentence analysis focuses on deconstructing a sentence until you have identified every constituent and every syntactic function realized by the constituents.
Broadly speaking, sentences can be broken down into three levels:
- The sentence as a whole
- A variable number of syntagmata (or phrases), including ones placed within other syntagmata
- Individual words
When analyzing a sentence, you identify each segment at a given level before moving on to the next level, moving from the sentence as a whole through the phrases and down to the individual words.
(Hint: You should always start by identifying the words that look like they’re semantically connected to one another so you’re aware how the words have formed groups.)
Constituents
A constituent is “the material that a segment is made from” (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 26).
This perspective sees language as a structure: How were the sentence’s “building blocks” combined into a single unit?
The best⁠-⁠known constituents are word classes, or parts of speech. Many people remember these from school.
There are also constituents that represent groups of words. These are called syntagmata, or phrases.
The words “syntagma” and “phrase” are synonymous, but the former is a technical term that avoids the potential misunderstandings and misuses found in everyday language. I use both in the overview below because of how dominant the term “phrase” remains within anglophone linguistics.
Syntagmata represent the different ways in which words can be grouped to create partial meanings within a sentence.
When analyzing, it’s also commonplace to count it as a syntagma when a segment only contains a single word. But to save time, you can just note the word itself in these cases.
Here are three main clauses featuring three syntagmata each, one per color:
- “Someone has done something.”
- “The elderly gentleman is eating a cold ice cream cone.”
- “One third of the students failed to prepare themselves for the exam.”
The words within each syntagma are clearly connected to one another. The groups make up partial meanings within the sentence.
Congruence is the principle that there can’t be grammatical contradictions within the same phrase.
For example, it would be wrong to write “a cold ice cream cones” as that phrase marks both the singular and the plural.
This principle is particularly important in other languages, including European languages other than English that use grammatical case and⁠/⁠or nouns in multiple genders.
Syntagmata are often named after word classes. For example, the syntagma “the elderly gentleman” is a noun phrase.
(See hierarchies within syntagmata and syntagmata below.)
Functions
It’s important to know what functions are because the internal anatomy of syntagmata is typically described by referring to those.
Functions represent a perspective that sees language as a system: Which roles are played by the “building blocks” within a sentence?
(Functions are often called grammatical relations in English.)
To describe this, you’ll need a different vocabulary than the one used for constituents. Here’s an overview based on Hoe and Nølke (2014, 10):
- Verbal function
- Subject
- Direct object
-
Adjuncts, including:
- Subject predicate
- Object predicate
- Indirect object
- Dative object
- Locative object
- Adverbials (bound and free)
- Free subject predicate
- Free subject predicate
- Free dative
To save space, these aren’t defined here. See chapters 4 and 5 in Hoe and Nølke (2014, 11⁠-⁠23). This list isn’t exhaustive.
Hierarchies within syntagmata
Syntagmata can be classified according to the hierarchical relationship between the segments within them:
-
Hypotaxis is when the segments are connected in a classic hierarchy. These phrases have a mandatory core, sometimes called a nucleus, which is more important than other segments within the phrase. Other segments vary in degree of importance. (A hypotactic syntagma is sometimes called a hypotagma.)
Examples include noun phrases, pronominal phrases, adjective phrases, and adverb phrases. -
Catataxis is when the segments are equal and mutually dependent. These phrases contain two mandatory elements (and no more) that can only build the phrase if they’re both present. (A catatactic syntagma is sometimes called a catatagma.)
Examples include nominal phrases, prepositional phrases, and conjunctional phrases. -
Parataxis is when there’s a coordinating relation between two or more elements, such as enumerations (“A, B, and C”). These phrases need to have at least two elements and one conjunction to bind the elements together, but you can expand this pattern with as many elements as you want. (A paratactic syntagma is called a paratagma.)
There are two subtypes of parataxis:- Syndetic parataxis expresses the coordinating relation between the elements in the group using a word (a coordinating conjunction).
- Asyndetic parataxis doesn’t express the relation between the elements in the group using a word (but may do so using punctuation, notably commas or semicolons).
It’s possible to have parataxis between two main clauses, such as: “You went into town, and I stayed at home.” This is called a sentence paratagma.
Verb phrases are an umbrella term for syntagmata that look like sentences (minus the subject). There are two types:
- Finite verb phrases realize their core, the verbal function, using a finite verb.
- Non⁠-⁠finite verb phrases realize the verbal function using a non⁠-⁠finite verb. They vary in terms of names and unique segments depending on which non⁠-⁠finite form is used.
Syntagmata
This section contains an overview of the most important syntagmata and their segments, based on Hoe and Nølke (2014, 41, 29⁠-⁠38).
See also constituents, functions, and hierarchies within syntagmata.
Overview
- Main clause
- Subordinate clause
- Verb phrases:
Main clause
- The largest unit within a sentence analysis. (The segment between two periods is called a period and may contain multiple main clauses.)
- Subject (function): Is typically realized by a noun phrase or non⁠-⁠finite verb phrase.
- Predicate (function): Everything that isn’t the subject. Is always realized by a finite verb phrase (constituent).
Examples:
-
“Henry is reading a book.”
(Main clause featuring a subject and predicate. The subject is realized by a proper noun. The predicate is realized by a finite verb phrase.) -
“A red rose has several symbolic meanings.”
(Main clause featuring a subject and predicate. The subject is realized by a noun phrase. The predicate is realized by a finite verb phrase. -
“Studying the night before an exam may not be ideal.”
(Main clause featuring a subject and predicate. The subject is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb phrase (gerund phrase). The predicate is realized by a finite verb phrase.)
Subordinate clause
- Within a sentence’s internal hierarchy, this segment is only found somewhere below the predicate of a main clause.
- Subordinate clauses are technically a special type of conjunctional phrase (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 33).
-
Conjunctional (function): A preliminary segment connecting the phrase to the rest of the sentence. Is realized by a subordinate conjunction (constituent).
Sometimes, a double meaning arises when the conjunctional function also makes up the clause’s subject. This is when it’s realized by a pronoun instead.
And, in some cases (depending on the language or level of formality), the conjunctional is left out entirely. - Subject (function): Like the subject in main clauses.
- Predicate (function): Like the predicate in main clauses.
Examples:
-
“We can’t believe [that they didn’t fix it sooner].”
(Subordinate clause with a conjunctional function, subject, and predicate. The conjunctional function is realized by a conjunction. The subject is realized by a pronoun. The predicate is realized by a finite verb phrase.) -
“The audience applauded the athletes [who had won medals].”
(Subordinate clause with a preliminary section featuring a double function, a conjunctional function and subject, as well as a predicate. The conjunctional and subject are realized by the pronoun who. The predicate is realized by a finite verb phrase.)
Below you will find the major verb phrases built around finite and non⁠-⁠finite verbs.
Verb phrases are where you’ll typically find major functions such as direct objects, indirect objects, and adverbials. The verbal function is the core of these phrases, but the other half of the nexus relation (the subject) is analyzed separately at a higher level, in main clauses and subordinate clauses.
This is what it means when the descriptions below say that these phrases are “analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses”. They have a verbal function as their core, but are otherwise analyzed like sentences (minus the subject).
Finite verb phrase
- Analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
-
Verbal (function): Is always realized by a finite verb (constituent), including complex tenses.
When it’s a complex tense the constituents are sometimes spread out within the phrase, making it necessary to mark them (and the function) at separate locations.
Example:
-
“Henry [reads a book].”
(Finite verb phrase with a verbal function and a direct object. The verbal function is realized by a finite verb. The object is realized by a noun phrase.)
Non⁠-⁠finite verb phrases
The range of phrases found in this category varies depending on which non⁠-⁠finite forms are available in a given language and how the phrases are constructed.
For example, Greek verbs have no infinitive among their non-finite forms and so cannot establish infinitive phrases, while the infinitive does exist in English, French, and German, among many others.
Always refer to a quality grammar book for information about specific syntagmata used in your language of interest.
Infinitive phrase
- Is analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
- Infinitive marker (function): Is always realized by an infinitive particle (constituent), such as the English word to in “to laugh”.
- Verbal (function): Is always realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb (constituent) in the infinitive.
-
“I am calling you [to tell you something important].”
(Infinitive phrase with an infinitive marker and verbal function. The marker is realized by the infinitive particle to. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the infinitive.)
Past participle phrase
- Is analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
- Verbal (function): Is always realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the past participle.
Example:
-
“[Excited by the announcement,] they contacted their families.”
(Past participle phrase with a verbal function. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the past participle.)
Present participle phrase
- Is analyzed the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
- Verbal (function): Is always realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle (constituent).
- Expands on the action performed by the sentence’s subject.
- Is usually used in “one of the adjectivistic functions, i.e., the attributive, apposition, and free predicative, but not the subject predicative. They are also used in absolute constructions” (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 36).
- In English, this type of phrase is easy to confuse with gerund phrases. Present participle phrases realize the kinds of functions that adjectives or adverbs do. Gerund phrases only realize the kinds of functions that nouns do.
Examples:
-
“[Knowing that it is illegal,] they went ahead and did it anyway.”
(Present participle phrase with a verbal function and a direct object. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle. The direct object is realized by a subordinate clause.) -
“The security guard, [having an uneasy feeling], pointed the flashlight towards the end of the hall.”
(Present participle phrase with a verbal function and a direct object. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle. The direct object is realized by a noun phrase.)
Gerund phrase
- Is analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
- Verbal (function): Is always realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle.
- In English, this type of phrase is easy to confuse with present participle phrases. Gerund phrases realize the kinds of functions that nouns do. Present participle phrases realize the kinds of functions that adjectives or adverbs do.
Examples:
-
“I like [creating visual art].”
(Gerund phrase with a verbal function and direct object. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle. The direct object is realized by a noun phrase. Note that the gerund phrase realizes the direct object of the sentence as a whole.) -
“[Playing MMORPGs] is a lot of fun.”
(Gerund phrase with a verbal function and direct object. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the present participle. The direct object is realized by a noun phrase with a single noun, the acronym MMORPGs. Note that the gerund phrase realizes the subject of the sentence as a whole.)
Perfect participle phrase
- Is analyzed in the same way as the contents of the predicate in main clauses.
- Verbal (function): Is always realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the perfect participle. Note that this is a composite form consisting of an auxiliary verb in the present participle and a main verb in the past participle.
Example:
-
“[Having finished the book the night before], she began typing a review for her blog.”
(Perfect participle phrase with a verbal function, direct object, and adverbial function. The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb in the perfect participle. The direct object is realized by a noun phrase, “the book”. The adverbial is also realized by a noun phrase, “the night before”.)
Below you will find the major hypotactic syntagmata:
Noun phrase
- Internal hierarchy: hypotaxis
- Typically used in the sentence’s valency.
- Core (function): Primarily realized by a noun (constituent) or other word classes understood to behave like nouns. The core is the most important segment in this phrase. It’s mandatory and limited to one per phrase. (This segment is sometimes called the nucleus.)
- Determiner (function): Adds additional information about the nucleus using articles, pronouns, or numerals. There are also “creative” determiners that are realized by syntagmata (such as “two kilos” in the phrase “two kilos of potatoes”). Limited to one segment per phrase.
- Pre⁠-⁠determiner (function): A less frequent segment that only occurs when there’s also a determiner. Is realized by certain words, such as “all” in the phrase “all my friends”. Limited to one segment per phrase.
- Attribute (function): Is especially realized by adjectives (constituent). In principle, there can be an infinite number of attributes within the phrase (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 29).
- Apposition (function): These are bound additions placed in the middle of the phrase. “A sort of middle ground between attributes and free predicatives” (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 29). They’re always pronounced as parenthetical additions and are usually placed next to the segment they’re bound to.
Examples:
-
“[A red rose] has several symbolic meanings.”
(Noun phrase with a determiner, attribute, and core. The determiner is realized by an indefinite article. The attribute is realized by an adjective. The core is realized by a noun.) -
“They didn’t understand [all the points].”
(Noun phrase with a pre⁠-⁠determiner, determiner, and core. The pre-determiner is realized by the pronoun all. The determiner is realized by the definite article. The core is realized by a noun.) -
“We visited [Odense, the native town of H.C. Andersen].”
(Noun phrase with a core and apposition. The core is realized by a proper noun. The apposition is realized by a noun phrase.)
Pronominal phrase
- Internal hierarchy: hypotaxis
- Core (function): Like noun phrases, but realized by a pronoun (constituent) instead. Limited to one segment per phrase. (This segment is sometimes called the nucleus.)
- Attribute (functon): Like noun phrases. Limited to one segment per phrase.
Example:
-
“I’ll bring [this here] with me.”
(Pronominal phrase with a core and post-positioned attribute. The core is realized by a demonstrative pronoun. The attribute is realized by an adverb.)
Adjective phrase
- Internal hierarchy: hypotaxis
- Core (function): Like noun phrases, but realized by an adjective (constituent) or other word class used in an adjective⁠-⁠like way. Limited to one segment per phrase. (This segment is sometimes called the nucleus.)
- Adverbial (function): Realized by a constituent that ‘nuances’ the meaning of the core, such as very or particularly.
- Adjunct (function): Is realized by a post⁠-⁠positioned prepositional phrase. Limited to one segment per phrase.
Examples:
-
“The painting is [very beautiful].”
(Adjective phrase with an adverbial function and core. The adverbial function is realized by an adverb. The core is realized by an adjective.) -
“The parents are [proud of their children].”
(Adjective phrase with a core and adjunct. The core is realized by an adjective. The adjunct is realized by a prepositional phrase.)
Adverb phrase
- Internal hierarchy: hypotaxis
- Core (function): Like noun phrases, but realized by an adverb (constituent). Limited to one segment per phrase. (This segment is sometimes called the nucleus.)
- Adverbial (function): Like adjective phrases.
Example:
-
“The bird sings [quite beautifully].”
(Adverb phrase with an adverbial function and core. The adverbial function is realized by an adverb. The core is realized by an adverb.)
Below you will find the major catatactic syntagmata:
Nominal phrase
- Internal hierarchy: catataxis
- These appear in the same parts of a sentence as noun phrases do.
- Determiner (function): Like noun phrases. Mandatory. Limited to one segment per phrase.
- Pre⁠-⁠determiner (function): Like noun phrases. Limited to one segment per phrase.
- Complement (function): Is always realized by a noun phrase. Mandatory. Limited to one segment per phrase.
This is sometimes used as an additional step when analyzing languages that strictly require that a determiner must always be present, such as French.
In other languages where such a step isn’t necessary, noun phrases are sometimes called nominal phrases. I haven’t seen real nominal phrases used in the analysis of sentences in Danish, English, or German.
Here’s how this step is taken in the analysis:
- The segments typically found within a noun phrase are split into two parts.
- The determiner and pre⁠-⁠determiner are placed within the nominal phrase.
- The nominal phrase’s complement is then realized by a noun phrase containing the remaining segments.
An example in French:
-
“[Les fans étrangers] sont très enthousiastes.”
English: “[The foreign fans] are very enthusiastic.”
(Nominal phrase with a determiner and complement. The determiner is realized by the definite article. The complement is realized by a noun phrase with no determiner.)
Prepositional phrase
- Internal hierarchy: catataxis
- Prepositional (function): Is always realized by a preposition (constituent).
- Complement (function): Is commonly realized by phrases based on nominal units such as noun phrases or nominal phrases, but it can also be realized by clauses, etc.
Example:
-
“The pirates buried the treasure [on a deserted island].”
(Prepositional phrase with a prepositional function and complement. The prepositional function is realized by a preposition. The complement is realized by a noun phrase.)
Conjunctional phrase
- Internal hierarchy: catataxis
- Conjunctional (function): Is realized by a subordinating conjunction (constituent). (For coordinating conjunctions, see paratactic syntagmata.)
- Complement (function): Like prepositional phrases.
Example:
-
“[Because we were stressed,] we went on holiday to relax.”
(Conjunctional phrase with a conjunctional function and complement. The conjunctional function is realized by a subordinating conjunction. The complement is realized by a main clause.)
Below you will find the major paratactic syntagmata:
Paratagma
- Internal hierarchy: parataxis
- Element (function): Is often numbered. The phrase requires at least two elements.
- Conjunctional (function): Is realized by a coordinating conjunction (constituent), such as and. This function can also be numbered if there are multiple ones within the phrase. The phrase requires at least one conjunctional function.
- “A paratagma can contain an infinite number of elements in principle. We note here that commas can also function as conjunctionals in enumerations.” (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 34; compare syndetic and asyndetic parataxis.)
Examples:
-
“I feel [hurt, disappointed, betrayed, and abandoned].”
(Paratagma with four elements and three conjunctional functions. Each element is realized by an adjective. The conjunctional functions are realized by two commas and a coordinating conjunction. The final Oxford comma doesn’t play a syntactic role. The word and is the final connector.) -
“There is a bird right behind [Wendy and her younger brother.]”
(Paratagma with two elements and one conjunctional function. The first element is realized by a proper noun. The second element is realized by a noun phrase. The conjunctional function is realized by the coordinating conjunction and.)
Sentence paratagma
- Identical to paratagmata, but each element is realized by a main clause.
- At the highest level of analysis, a sentence paratagma results in two sentence analyses combined into one.
Examples:
-
“Joan is playing drums and Jacob is playing electric guitar.”
(Sentence paratagma with two elements and one conjunctional function. The two elements are each realized by a main clause. The conjunctional function is realized by the coordinating conjunction and.) -
“I tried reaching the bus in time, but it was too late.”
(Sentence paratagma with two elements and one conjunctional function. The two elements are each realized by a main clause. The conjunctional function is realized by the coordinating conjunction but.)
Nexus relations
A nexus is a special relation connecting the verbal function and subject (functions) in a sentence. See Hoe and Nølke (2014, 29). There exist two types:
- Primary nexus: The verbal function is realized by a finite verb. The subject is made explicit in the sentence. This is especially seen in main clauses and subordinate clauses.
- Secondary nexus: The verbal function is realized by a non⁠-⁠finite verb. The subject isn’t made explicit in the sentence but is an implicit part of the sentence’s valency. This is especially seen in non⁠-⁠finite verb phrases.
Certain languages, such as Greek, leave out the subject when there’s a primary nexus but the subject is realized by a personal pronoun and can be identified from the verb’s conjugation or the general context. These pronoun-dropping languages are known as pro⁠-⁠drop languages.
This phenomenon is typically limited to a specific word class. When the subject is realized by other word classes, it still has to be made explicit.
Valency grammar
A sentence’s valency represents a special relation between the main verbal function and the nominal segments (or functions) associated with it, which are called agents. See Herslund 2020b.
The number of agents depends on the meaning of the verb. “One might say that the meaning of the verb is incomplete without these agents” (Hoe and Nølke 2014, 11).
-
Monovalent verbs aren’t transitive. They only have one agent associated with them: the subject.
Examples: “I am dying,” “The bear is sleeping.”
(You can’t say “I am dying an exam” or “The bear is sleeping the child an ice cream cone”. But, in literary language, you can say things like “I am dying a slow death” or “The bear is sleeping the deepest of sleeps.” These are typically exceptions.) -
Divalent verbs have two agents. There are two different scenarios:
-
The subject plus a direct object (functions), meaning that the verb is transitive.
Example: “Henry is reading a book.” -
The subject plus an adjunct (functions), meaning that the verb is intransitive.
Example: “The hunter is aiming at the wolf.”
-
The subject plus a direct object (functions), meaning that the verb is transitive.
-
Trivalent verbs have three agents: the subject, direct object, and adjunct.
Example: ”The man gave his wife an anniversary gift.”
Language in context: Pragmatics
The linguistic discipline of pragmatics describes language in practice, meaning within communicative contexts.
This is a vast field with many sub⁠-⁠specializations. The following branches may be of particular interest to language learners:
- Communication theory, especially intercultural communication
- Text linguistics, especially theories of text genres (as these often vary from one society to another.)
- Translation studies, especially the notions of equivalence, translation strategies, literary translation, functional translation, and Skopos theory
- Interpreting studies
Linguistic variety
No language in the world exists in a frozen, permanent, universal state of being.
The language researcher Rosina Lippi⁠-⁠Green (2004, 293) has described standard language ideology as “a bias toward an abstracted, idealized, non⁠-⁠varying spoken language that is imposed and maintained by dominant institutions” (emphasis in original).
This and other normative patterns of thought are widespread but don’t align with the empirical reality of language in use.
Here are some terms to describe different types of linguistic variation based on Gadet (2003, 9⁠–⁠12):
- Diachronic variation: Variation across time. New and old ‘versions’ of a language. Consider Old English (ca. 500⁠–⁠1100 AD), Middle English (ca. 1100⁠–⁠1500 AD), Early Modern English (ca. 1500⁠–⁠1800 AD), and Late Modern English (ca. 1800s to the present).
- Diatopic variation: Variation across space –⁠ geographic variation. These can often be grouped into major dialect areas, such as the southern dialects of the USA (Southern American English) or the northern dialects of England.
- Diastratic variation: Variation based on social hierarchies. Sociolects.
- Diaphasic variation: Variation determined by situations. One example is registers, where certain vocabulary or grammatical constructs are chosen rather than others so as to signal a greater or lesser level of formality and⁠/⁠or intimacy.
Translation published 13 June 2023.
Bibliography
-
“Sproget –⁠ hverdagens mirakel.” Aarhus University Press.
https://en.unipress.dk /udgivelser /s/sproget/ -
“Hypotakse.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /hypotakse -
“Paratakse.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /paratakse -
“Meget mystisk middelalderbog har været kilde til undren i århundreder.” Forskerzonen.
https://videnskab.dk /kultur-samfund /meget-mystisk-middelalderbog-har-vaeret-kilde-til-undren-i-aarhundreder -
“La diversité linguistique et la variation.” La variation sociale en français, 9⁠–⁠12. L'Éssentiel. Paris: Ophrys. -
“Halvvokal.” Den Store Danske
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /halvvokal -
“Lette pronomeners placering: klise og topologisk integritet.” Ny Forskning i Grammatik 18, 61⁠–⁠80.
https://doi.org /10.7146 /nfg.v19i18.23668 -
“Aktant.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /aktant -
“Ord.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /ord -
“Register (stilleje og tone).” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /register _-_stilleje _og_tone -
“Valens (sprogvidenskabeligt begreb).” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /valens _-_sprogvidenskabeligt_begreb -
Sætningens fysiognomi –⁠ indføring i syntaktisk analyse..” Edited by Merete Birkelund. (Pré)publications 201.
https://pure.au.dk /portal /en/persons /merete-birkelund(23d33241-3cd4-481e-9e2e-d47d2d9a35db) /publications /saetningens-fysiognomi(c97916ca-87dc-49cd-9d9a-857c705e15ef).html
“ -
“Katatakse.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /katatakse -
“Language ideology and language prejudice.” Language in the USA, 289⁠–⁠304. Cambridge University Press.
https://www.cambridge.org /core /books/language-in-the-usa /language-ideology-and-language-prejudice /1B854A9FD2D520A6F670A24376A1BCF2 -
“Dansk –⁠ historie.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /dansk_-_historie -
”Dansk grammatik –⁠ turbokursus for fremmedsprogsstuderende.” -
“Agglutinerende sprog.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /agglutinerende_sprog -
“Analytiske sprog.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /analytiske_sprog -
“Syntetiske sprog.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /syntetiske_sprog -
“Ytring.” Den Store Danske.
https://denstoredanske.lex.dk /ytring